CareerSteer – careers test for career choice www.careersteer.org
Informal mentoring, white collar and blue, in a British city. ABSTRACT A cross-sectional survey concentrating on informal mentoring was completed by 404 workers of various occupations in Norwich. As in previous studies, mentoring was correlated with positive outcomes for employees. Kram's (1985) factorial model of mentoring relationships was supported, except in the case of the blue collar workers; differences inherent in the blue collar sample, however, provided some evidence in favour of a causal link between mentoring and the outcomes. As in other studies, gender differences were insignificant except in the expected areas of income and, in this study, job satisfaction. Levinson's (1978) characterisation of mentors as hierarchical superiors was supported. A correlation was found between potential proteges' social orientation at work and the probability of becoming mentored. Formal mentoring was not perceived as influential in careers, although it was considered to be useful for induction purposes. INTRODUCTION Mentoring, for the purposes of this study, can be understood as a relationship - often interactive - between an older, more experienced person within an organisation and a younger, less experienced one, serving a variety of developmental functions for both of them (Kram, 1985). Levinson's model of adult career development (1978) postulates the likely influence of a mentor during the stage of early adulthood; one aspect of this relationship is that it should become more equal as the protege acquires skills. Adherents of Erikson's epigenetic theory of ego development (1959) would also expect older successful persons, in a period of 'generativity', to actively want to be mentors, passing on the fruits of their experience. Kram, the leading theorist of mentoring relationships, postulates two superordinate mentoring dimensions, career oriented and psychosocial. The career oriented dimension derives from the functions of sponsorship, coaching, protection, exposure, and the provision of challenging work for proteges; they largely relate to experience, rank, and influence. Psychosocial functions comprise role modeling, counselling, acceptance, confirmation, and friendship; these are said to foster "a clear sense of professional identity" as well as aiding confidence and competence. "Relationships that provide both kinds of functions are characterized by greater intimacy and commitment and are viewed as more indispensable, more critical to development, and more exclusive than other relationships at work. Relationships that only provide career functions are characterized by less intimacy and are valued primarily for the instrumental ends that they serve in an organizational context. More often than not, relationships provide a subset of the possible mentoring functions, and, in general, career functions are more prevalent" (Kram, 1985). Other models have been postulated. Schein, for example, provides a model (1978) concentrating on roles more than functions; these include the roles of sponsor, role model, teacher/coach/trainer, leader, protector, opener of doors, and talent developer. Factor analytic studies by Noe (1988) and Scandura (1992), however, have broadly supported Kram's model. Dreher and Ash (1990) found that business graduates experiencing extensive mentoring relationships reported receiving more promotions, had higher incomes, and were more satisfied with their pay and benefits than were those experiencing less extensive mentoring relationships. No significant gender relationships were identified, except for the seemingly inevitable income differential. Other studies have reached similar conclusions (e.g. Fagenson, 1989). Dreher and Ash (1990) have noted the problems in trying to identify causal relationships. 'High-fliers' may be adopted more readily as proteges; Whiteley et al (1992) discuss social origins as a possible variable. In other words, mentoring may not be a cause of success. Interpersonal attributes are also thought to have a bearing upon the formation of mentoring relationships. On the side of the mentor, Levinson (1978) and Clawson (1980) considered a hierarchical superior to be typical, Whiteley et al (1992) considered the role of the manager within the organisation, whilst Olian et al (1988) found that differences in managers' interpersonal skills significantly affected protege attraction to the potential mentor, organisational integration of the mentor only proving significant in the event of weaker interpersonal skills in the mentor. Considering proteges, Olian et al found that younger proteges were more likely to be attracted to potential mentors. Fagenson (1992) found that proteges had significantly greater needs for power and achievement than nonproteges. Another point is that research tends to be American, is usually based upon studies of managers (often alumni of graduate business schools), and tends to emphasise formal mentoring (e.g. Noe, 1988). Although some studies (Berlew and Hall, 1966; Clawson, 1980) have considered the 'facilitation of organisational socialisation' - or induction - American mentoring studies generally relate to major career influences THE SCOPE OF THIS STUDY Perhaps it is not entirely coincidental that a British review of the mentoring literature (Collin, 1979) asked the research question of whether or not there were typologies of development for non-managers, and if any mentoring roles existed within the formal hierarchy. She proposed that the mentor personifies the company's 'psychostructure' and acts the midwife in the process of socialisation. One study of MBAs suggested that informal mentoring was more effective than a formal system, although not necessarily as fair (Iles and Mabey, 1993). If we take into account the point that mentoring systems have been adopted less frequently in Britain than in the U.S.A. (banking and further education would appear to be exceptions), it would appear to be unrealistic to make a direct comparison with American models. Given this inability to test directly whether or not such systems work similarly across national cultures, the research may usefully study different phenomena. It is not suggested, however, that differences ininternational culture are insignificant (c.f. Hofstede, 1980). Rather that, contrary to Roche's (1979) U.S. survey of 1,250 executives, where two thirds had a mentor or sponsor in their early careers, and Kram's assumption of the development of subordinates as a mainstream activity within American managerial tasks, the current British industrial climate is not conducive to the development of subordinates beyond the parameters of obvious short-term needs. Formal mentoring systems are rare; their usage will be looked at, however. This study concentrates upon informal mentoring, over a broad(if not thoroughly representative) sample of the working population within an English city. A factor analysis of the typology of mentoring relationships will test Kram's (1985) model in this rather different setting. The only measure of intrapersonal attributes was the question about attitudes to work ('social orientation'). This was applied to Judith Coles' idea of receptivity to mentoring. HYPOTHESES 1 That mentored individuals should have more job satisfaction, higher wages and greater career opportunities than non-mentored individuals. 2 That gender differences will not prove significant (with the probable exception of income differentials). 3 That the levels of mentoring will vary greatly between professional and working class careers. 4 That official mentoring will not appear to be widespread, and that it will not be viewed as particularly beneficial by its recipients. Further questions to be asked: Are Kram's factor analytical qualities valid in Britain? Will the mentor typically be a hierarchical superior? RESEARCH DESIGN AND METHODOLOGY The questionnaire A correlational cross-sectional survey (see Appendix A), this distinguished the following demographic groups: gender, occupation, age, and income. Three attitudinal scales (questions 2.1 - 2.3) were cruded tests of job satisfaction, subjective attainment (c.f. Lawrence, 1984, on the 'social clock'), and social orientation at work. The latter, as well as measuring affiliation needs, also served as an indirect indicator of receptivity to mentoring. Those who considered themselves to have been greatly influenced in their careers were asked to continue with some details of the mentoring relationship and then a global measure of mentoring practices. Questions represented different factors postulated by Kram (1985); see Appendix B. The measure was drawn from Dreher and Ash (1990), who themselves selected items used by Noe (1988) and Whiteley et al. (1988). Minor amendments were made to assist comprehension by a wider range of respondents than in the previous studies. The researcher added a 19th. question, from the Noe study, in order to include an essential part of coaching. Brief questions on formal mentoring were asked of all respondents, with room for comments. It seemed likely that formal mentoring would be scarce in the British context, so little space was devoted to this issue. The issue was placed at the rear of the questionnaire to avoid influencing choices made about informal influences. The sample Questionnaires were distributed via simple random sampling of the working population of Norwich. The guiding priorities were to gain information speedily and over a broad range of occupations and incomes. Accessibility of subjects within the time and personal budget available meant that a truly representative range was out of the question. Random sampling was therefore simple rather than proportional, although still intending to be numerically representative. The Census of 1991 considered the population of Norwich, aged between 16 and 65, to be 76,864. The working population of Norwich will be less when unemployment is taken into account, but with the well-known phenomenon of commuting - into Norfolk's only city - the working population probably ranges between one and two hundred thousand people. Krejcie and Morgan (1970) recommend a sample size of 384 for a population of between 75,000 and a million; Roscoe (1975) recommends a maximum of 500 for such studies. One city was chosen to avoid the confounding variable of economic fluctuations across geographic areas. Certain points should be noted about the context, however. The city of Norwich is primarily populated by caucasian people; indeed, it has the dubious distinction of being called the 'last white city' by some neo-fascists. Racial factors such as those considered by Thomas (1990) therefore could not be covered; arguably, this excludes another potentially confounding variable. A distinction of the county of Norfolk is that its relatively slow pace of life is popular and the area is therefore often known as the 'graveyard of ambition'. The implications of this for such factors as mobility and promotion should be considered when considering the generalisability of this study to other parts of Britain. Data collection 901 questionnaires were distributed, 438 (48.61%) returning, of which 3 were discarded as unusable. 404 arrived on time for analysis. Questionnaires were distributed personally by the researcher in the case of individuals and very small groups, otherwise via managers and other officials. With the exception of a few organisations which made alternative arrangements, all individuals received a questionnaire with (attached by paper clip), an envelope, stamped first class and printed with the researcher's Norwich address. Where requested, the researcher would summarise verbally the contents of the questionnaire; in larger organisations, the researcher requested distribution to a range of hierarchical levels. Follow-up calls were only made at the suggestion of questionnaire recipients (a rare occurrence). Questionnaires went out in the following proportions, essentially based upon accessibility: banks 20%; post office 12%; social workers 12%; fire service 11%; diverse civil servants 10%; hospital staff 6%; police 6%; education 4% estate agents 2%; recruitment agencies 2%; voluntary\charitable organisations 2%; railway employees 1%; others, including self-employed people, taxi drivers, sales and bar staff, various professionals, and retired individuals 12%. Of the 404 questionnaires analysed, the approximate proportions were: banks 26.5%; hospital\education\social work 14.5%; post office 9.5%; police 8.5%; civil servants 8.5%; fire 5% rail 2%; miscellaneous 25.5%. Particularly for the purposes of factor analysis, the occupational groups were conflated into blue collar (125) and white collar (279) workers. Piloting Six pilot questionnaires were distributed to friends from a variety of work backgrounds. Various criticisms of format led to minor changes in the questionnaire, including the inclusion of HND as an academic qualification level. The relevance of the subject matter to particular workers was questioned. Of particular interest was the idea of 'receptivity to mentoring' as a likely influence on the take-up of mentoring. Judith Coles, one of the pilot respondents, raised this as an issue; as discussed, other researchers have looked at rather specific attributes influencing the mentoring process. The researcher decided to use the question about social orientation at work as an indirect measure of this; those who saw work as divorced from social aspects were likely to be the people who, according to Coles, tended to pass over potential mentors. Lack of time prevented a thorough pilot distribution of questionnaires. An analysis of the first 50 mentoring scales for those with mentoring relationships, indicated strong internal consistency; a subsequent Cronbach Alpha reliability test on the total 177 cases factor analysed gave a coefficient of .8967 on the first 18 items of the mentoring factor scales, .9027 with the 19th. question added. Consistency of the questions on formal mentoring was highly unsatisfactory (.1595). The attitudinal measures were poor (.5963), although the removal of the social orientation scale from the calculations gave the fair result of .6350. Analysis methods Factor analysis was used on the mentoring behaviour scales, studying all of those with mentors and also groups divided into blue and white collar occupations and also by gender. The direct method of analysis was Principal Components, with an oblique rotation technique (Oblimin). Kaiser's criterion (Eigenvalue > 1) was chosen for deciding upon the number of factors to be extracted, supported by the scree test (Cattell, 1978). For clarity, it was decided to limit analysis of coefficients to a minimum of .5. Structure matrices were usually chosen for analysis (a pattern matrice was used in the female category because it resembled the number of factors in the original statistics); these appear in Appendix H. Non-parametric methods were generally chosen for tests involving Likert scales. Although not based on interval data, it was felt reasonable to apply parametric testing to income given a relatively normal distribution. Similar considerations allowed the use of parametric testing on the aggregated scores of the mentor behaviour scales, which served as a measure of the quality of mentoring. This dependent variable will be referred to subsequently as 'addfacts'. Additional mentoring independent variables were the existence of mentoring or otherwise, the number of mentors, and the duration of the most important of these relationships. RESULTS 1 That mentored individuals should have more job satisfaction, higher wages and greater career opportunities than non-mentored individuals. This was supported overwhelmingly; those individuals who had been mentored differed from those who had not in terms of income (Kruskal-Wallis p. <.0001), job satisfaction (p <.0002) and subjective estimates of career level achievement (p <.0002). It should be noted, however, that the same statistical test uncovered similar relationships between academic qualifications and the dependent variables (income p <.0001; job satisfaction p <.005). Although this test failed to find a significant relationship with career level (chi square 8.4451 p <.1333), the Sign test did (p <.05 2-tailed). Unsurprisingly, qualifications and mentoring correlated (Spearman r= .0946; p <.05 1-tailed). Levels of perceived attainment varied interestingly, however, when distribution was examined in the light of qualifications. O level standard respondents appear to have fared the worst (Appendix E). Those with no qualifications (Appendix F), although clearly having the expected large proportion who have failed to achieve, have a higher mean than those with 'A' levels or higher (Appendix G) and have outstripped them in terms of 'over-achieving'. The quality of mentoring, as measured by the summation of factorial item scores ('addfact'), did not correlate significantly with income (Pearson), but was related to job satisfaction (Spearman .2491, p <.001) and to subjective attainment (Spearman .2009, p <.01). The number of mentors produced a similar set of relationships (Spearman: n.s.; r=.2491, p <.001; r=.2009, p <.001). 2 That gender differences will not prove significant (with the probable exception of income differentials). As Appendix D shows, males and females responded in almost equal numbers. As expected, there was a significant gender difference in income (independent t-test p <.001). Gender also correlated with job satisfaction (Mann-Whitney p <.001); the expected levels measure was not significant. The hypothesis as a whole is supported by the available evidence. Male and female respondents did not differ significantly in terms of 'addfact' (independent t-test), number of mentors (Mann-Whitney) or in the proportion of those mentored (Mann-Whitney; statistics in Appendix D). The relationship between gender and affiliation, as represented by the social orientation measure, was not significant (Mann-Whitney). The results were supported by separate analyses of mentored and non-mentored individuals. 3 That the levels of mentoring will vary greatly between professional and working class careers. The null hypothesis may not be rejected here. Of the 404 respondents, 125 were blue collar workers, 279 white collar. Of the 179 respondents (44.31%) who claimed to have had significant personal influences in their lives, 56 were blue collar (44.8%) and 123 were white collar (44.09%). The differences between the occupation categories on quality of mentoring (addfactors) and duration of the most important relationship were insignificant (Mann-Whitney). Blue collar workers claimed to have had more mentors (Mann-Whitney, p <.001). Interestingly, whilst white collar workers were generally better qualified academically (Mann-Whitney p <.0001), blue collar workers were better paid (Kolmogorov-Smirnov p <.02 2- tailed). Differences pertaining to job satisfaction and subjective expectations proved insignificant. Of particular interest for its implications for hypothesis 1 is that blue collar workers with no academic qualifications, who had also reached above their expected level of attainment, claimed to have had significant personal career influences (Spearman, p <.02 2-tailed). 4 That official mentoring will not appear to be widespread, and that it will not be viewed as particularly beneficial by its recipients. Of the 404 cases, 61 (15.09%) had had formal 'mentors'; another 61 had had 'buddies' or some other such allocated assistance. Correlations between these categories and perceived usefulness were not significant; respondents' opinions appear to have been supported by insignificant correlations with income, job satisfaction and career level assessments (Spearman). A negative correlation with the existence of informal mentors occurred in both cases (r = -.1249 p <.02, r = -.0984 p <.05; Spearman 2-tailed). Comments from those who had found these formal roles useful generally revealed that they were essentially induction procedures, run for a few weeks or months, rather than ongoing career assistance. This seemed particularly widespread in the blue collar occupations, and rather reflects the view of the British commentator, Audrey Collin, that the mentor is an agent of socialisation. Are Kram's higher order factors valid in Britain? The factor analyses conducted in this study tend to support Kram's general grouping of factors into psychosocial and career oriented higher order factors. The initial statistics for the factorised group as a whole were as follows: Eigenvalues % of variance cumulative % VC1 7.14827 37.6 37.6 VC10 2.14487 11.3 48.9 VC11 1.32133 7 55.9 VC12 1.05072 5.5 61.4 After rotation, the first factor contained 5 psychosocial elements (mainly 'counselling') and 2 apparently career-oriented elements. The latter, however, comprise sharing of the mentor's history with the protege (question 14) and similar attitudes and values (18); these particular elements, chosen by Noe (1988) to represent the coaching and role model factors could quite easily be interpreted as being fundamentally psychosocial in function. The second factor comprises 4 career oriented elements (3 exposure/visibility and 1 challenging assignments). Factor three comprises 6 career-oriented elements and 2 psychosocial elements. Again, classification seems to be the source of the apparent discontinuity: the 'counselling' role involves discussions of competence and promotion (question 13), with 'acceptance/confirmation' being encouragement to behave in new ways on the job' (16). Interpreting these as career oriented rather than psychosocial does not seem to be constraining facts in favour of theory unduly. The fourth factor comprises three career functions. Factors correlation matrix: Factor 1 Factor 2 Factor 3 Factor 4 Factor 1 1.00000 Factor 2 .23823 1.00000 Factor 3 -.47601 -.28740 1.00000 Factor 4 .28727 .32986 -.24768 1.00000 The strong negative correlation between factor 1 - psychosocial - and factor 3, a strongly career-oriented factor, are also very suggestive of Kram's higher order division. When the white collar occupational segment was studied independently, Kram's higher order functions were supported with even greater clarity. Eigenvalues % of variance cumulative % VC1 6.30568 33.2 33.2 VC10 2.31200 12.2 45.4 VC11 1.35617 7.1 52.5 VC12 1.19400 6.3 58.8 VC13 1.01946 5.4 64.1 Factor 1 comprised 4 psychosocial elements, with question 14 again; another apparently career-oriented element is question 18, pertaining to similar attitudes and values. The 5 elements in Factor 2 seem to cover coaching and advancement; the inclusion of questions 13 and 16 again do not seem to contradict this. Factor 3 is career oriented, comprising exposure and visibility (3 elements) and challenging assignments (1). Factor four comprises two role model elements, the fifth factor two protection elements. The Blue Collar category, however, shows a remarkably different topology. The initial statistics are as follows: Question Eigenvalue % of variance cumulative % VC3 7.14418 55 55 VC6 1.12195 8.6 63.6 Correlation -.56988 Both factors, as shown in Appendix H, contain a mixture of psychosocial and career-oriented elements (in Kram's formulation); the breadth of the mixture does not suggest blurring of interpretive classification. The nine elements of Factor 2 all feature amongst the thirteen in Factor 3; all are negative correlations, presumably indicating bi-polar relationships. Although gender divisions show fewer changes from the topology of the whole sample, differences do exist: Initial statistics follow for the men. Question Eigenvalue % of variance cumulative % 1 7.43755 39.1 39.1 10 2.20094 11.6 50.7 11 1.35305 7.1 57.9 12 1.07563 5.7 63.5 13 1.01525 5.3 68.9 Factor one has 2 unambiguous career-oriented elements, role model (question 17, example of how things should be done) and sponsorship (7, promotion of career interests). Factor two is exactly the same as that for the whole factorised sample. Factor three is very similar (minus question 8; plus 10 and 7). Factor four retains the 'protection' elements, 7 having been subsumed into the previous factor. Factor 5 contains two career-oriented elements (6 and 8). As in the main matrix, there is a large negative correlation between factors one and three. Factor correlation matrix: Factor 1 Factor 2 Factor 3 Factor 4 Factor 5 Factor 1 1.00000 Factor 2 .21636 1.00000 Factor 3 -.49934 -.21376 1.00000 Factor 4 .21271 .18390 -.16940 1.00000 Factor 5 .23983 .01944 -.21288 .05559 1.00000 106 of the 123 blue collar workers (86%) were men. Although causation is unclear, the relationship between worktype and gender (Chi-square .45894, p >.00001) suggests that the nature of the blue collar jobs may be an influential variable in explaining the difference in factor 1, given the great disparity of this category from all other factor analyses, the even gender spread in the study as a whole and the solid adherence of the white collar sample to the sample norm. A discussion of the qualitative responses to formal mentoring (hypothesis 4) is supportive of this argument. Initial statistics for the female category are as follows: Question Eigenvalue % of variance cumulative % 1 7.04670 37.1 37.1 10 2.33318 12.3 49.4 11 1.39170 7.3 56.7 12 1.26948 6.7 63.4 13 1.06172 5.6 69 Factor correlation matrix: Factor 1 Factor 2 Factor 3 Factor 4 Factor 5 Factor 1 1.00000 Factor 2 .23175 1.00000 Factor 3 .09018 .03620 1.00000 Factor 4 -.42860 -.34951 -.10744 1.00000 Factor 5 -.07441 -.01403 -.04889 .05991 1.00000 The first two factors, responsible for half the variance covered by the large Eigenvalues, are similar to the overall mentored sample. Differences occur in the lower factors (Appendix H holds the details). Will the mentor typically be a hierarchical superior? This was generally the case: line managers / supervisors 49.2% more senior managers 20.3% older / more experienced colleagues 15.8% colleagues generally 5.6% very senior managers (e.g. directors) 2.8% others (e.g. consultants, wives) 6.2% As the line chart in Appendix C indicates, it is generally within the line manager's ability to provide the best quality mentoring (by the addfacts measure). Additional results The social orientation measure correlates with the existence or otherwise of a mentoring relationship, as predicted by Coles' theory of receptivity to mentoring (Mann-Whitney p <.001). DISCUSSION The support for the first hypothesis, that mentoring is related to outcomes, reflects much of the literature referred to earlier. This result appears to have generalised from American studies of formal mentoring to a primarily informal system - perhaps this is rather an oxymoron - in Britain. It is not surprising that factor analysis reveals a closer adherence by white collar occupations to Kram's (1985) dichotomy of career orientated and psychosocial factors; this classification has more in common with the American respondents. Although the blue collar category was originally included to examine class differences (which appeared insubstantial in the light of hypothesis 3), the results pertaining to it may well have a bearing on a regular problem of cross-sectional studies: the direction of causation. This perennial is of course suggested by the correlation between qualifications and outcomes; have qualifications or accompanying impressions of being a 'high- fliers' attracted mentors. Whilst this may be a confounding variable in the white-collar occupations, this appears to be less likely in the blue-collar category, where a less academically qualified contingent seems not to differ significantly in mentoring levels. The correlation between expected levels of attainment and claims of significant personal career influences, when pertaining to those without any academic qualifications, does rather suggest that 'who you know matters more than what you know' at certain stages of some careers. Unfortunately, no such additional evidence allows such commentary to be applied to the relationship between mentoring quality (as measured by 'addfacts') and subjective measures. Results relating to gender differences appear to reflect the American studies. No differences in mentoring outcomes occurred; the usual income differential, however, was also accompanied by one relating to job satisfaction. No great differences in mentoring experiences emerged from the factor analysis. Differences in the male responses to factor analysis probably reflect the situation in blue-collar occupations. A difference occurs in the topology of the mentoring, although not in non- factorial measures. Kram's factors do not appear to cross the class barrier. The resultant 'feel' of the analysis (c.f. Child, 1990, on the reliance on subjective judgement relative to other statistical methods) is one of a very personal attention to a protege's advancement; perhaps division between advancement and interpersonal issues is more characteristic of white collar work places. This may be interpreted as supporting Kram's assertion (1985) that relationships providing both both kinds of functions are particularly beneficial for development. The complete blurring of definitions in the blue collar classification means, however, that the model has no construct validity in this domain and should not, therefore, be used for extrapolations here. Kram's model is supported generally, however; its extension beyond both country of origin and the usual respondent population is impressive. The strong negative correlations between strong psychosocial and career-oriented factor clusters are particularly indicative of her superordinate factors. Her claim (1985) that career functions are generally more prevalent may be domain specific, however, given the formal systems in the U.S. In this study, Factor 1, with the largest share of the variance by far, has a predominance of psychosocial loadings. British employment - if the Norwich survey is representative - does not appear to support formal mentoring in terms of influencing careers beyond organisational socialisation (which would appear to be primarily task-related). Given the research on efficacy of formal and informal systems (Iles and Mabey, 1993), this may not be problematic. Many of the recipients of formal mentoring did seem to appreciate its value as a tool of induction. It just does not comprise the general career influence portrayed by Kram and supports the assertion that British employers are only likely to organise systems for meeting short-term needs. Whilst Iles and Mabey cast doubts upon the fairness of informal allocation, this study would suggest that the likelihood of becoming mentored lies somewhat in the personality, or possibly conception of work, of the would-be protege. A measure of social orientation that was designed to look at gender differences in affiliation needs (not significant) was also used as an indirect measure of receptivity to mentoring. This is supportive of Judith Coles' observations: her own mentor, although invaluable to her, was not 'discovered' by several of her colleagues; when she herself became a mentor, some used her experience, others 'did not want to know'. In terms of mentor characteristics, the pre-eminence of hierarchical superiority is in line with Levinson's (1978) conception of the mentoring role. SOME CRITICISMS OF THE CURRENT STUDY Textual problems One of the flaws in the questionnaire itself was the original, ambiguous, classification of the global mentoring scales questions by Noe (1988). As noted earlier, the respondents' answers generally clustered meaningfully; Noe's categorisations sometimes lacked validity, failing to measure that which they purported to measure. In making alternative interpretations, the researcher may be accused of, at worst, constraining the facts to fit the theory, or - still problematic - making comparison with previous results rather difficult. Future replication is not a problem, however, given the explanation in the text of which elements were re- interpreted. The researcher's own questions left something to be desired. These were of low reliability. A proper pilot study was not conducted (a result of temporal and fiscal pressures). The job satisfaction measure was so crude as to be unworthy of debate relating to theory. The only excuse that may be offered is that the need to study the other focal points without producing an unwieldy questionnaire rather encouraged this dabbling. The use of an affiliative measure - and a riskily colloquial one at that - to indirectly measure receptivity to mentoring was of dubious validity. It really covered attitudes to work. It was shown to lack consistency and further study would be needed to see what is the underlying factor correlating with take-up of mentors. Some textual errors succeeded in surviving the small pilot scheme. At one point, the text refers to influences on careers; more than one respondent pointed out that some influences are malevolent, the very opposite of the benevolent mentoring under examination. A worse error was the 'definitely ot at all' at the crucial stage of the study where the respondent decided on whether or not he or she had been mentored. Fortunately, most respondents underwent the same Gestalt interpretation as the pilot proofreaders (perhaps being used to Likert scales); most of those who did follow the instruction literally gave subsequent answers which explained where they stood, but this error was certainly unhelpful and led to discards of questionnaires. Certain areas of study were discontinued. Data to be used to examine changes in mentoring over time and age-related qualities of mentoring relationships were rendered inaccessible by the database files becoming corrupt; the researcher had made the mistake of loading a software update immediately it arrived, rather than trying it out on a fresh piece of work. This technical problem also led to an inability to count elements arising from qualitative responses (e.g. how many formal mentors were seen as useful in the realm of induction). Analysis problems Strictly speaking, significance levels should have been determined before statistical tests were conducted, rather than using the readout from a computer program. Given more time, the researcher would have consulted about the usage, interpretation and presentation of factor analysis results. No explanation was given for the choice of an oblique rather than an orthogonal rotation technique. Similarly, no sensible rationale lay behind the choice of structure over pattern matrices. Worse was the switch to a pattern matrice when analysing the female sample, making comparisons with other topologies a rather dubious undertaking. The idea of doing this for closer adherence to the original statistics defeats the object of the rotation, to give derived solutions. Sampling The smallness of the mentored sample of 'blue collar' workers, coupled with the reliance on post office workers, firefighters, and police officers, may have biased the results in favour of workers in large (and generally uniformed) organisations. More generally, the overall sample may well have been representative of the city's working population, but the convenience sampling did not give random access to different levels of employee status. As noted earlier in the study, Norwich was not an ideal population for hoping to extrapolate to other areas of the country, let alone elsewhere. Whilst the method of data collection coincided with a good response rate for questionnaires without follow-up (Bailey, 1978), the use of both personal distribution and first class mail for completed questionnaires makes it hard to ascertain which was the effective variable. Also, the distribution process could only be replicated fully by the researcher. CONCLUSION Mentoring is related to outcomes; this result appears to have generalised from American studies primarily of formal mentoring to informal mentoring in Britain. Similarly, Kram's (1985) model of the nature of mentoring relationships is supported. The domain into which her model does not generalise - the blue collar occupational category - provides useful evidence about the more general outcomes. The blue collar sample did not differ significantly in general outcomes, only in factorial responses about the mentoring process, where the distinction between career-oriented and psychosocial factors seemed non-existent. Similar correlations existing between mentoring and outcome measures, without the confounding variable of academic qualifications, are supportive of a theory of causal direction in which mentoring influences progress (as opposed to being a product of other forces, merely accelerating an existing momentum). Gender findings replicate those of other studies. Differences are insignificant, except in the expected areas of income and, in this study, job satisfaction. Male differences were probably attributable to phenomena within the blue collar sample, where men predominated. In terms of mentor characteristics, the pre-eminence of hierarchical superiority is in line with Levinson's (1978) conception of the mentoring role. Formal mentoring appears well suited to induction, its current usage in Britain. This study suggests a relationship between the up-take of informal mentoring, with its broader implications, and the receptivity of the potential protege; social orientation at work was the measure used. Recommendations Whilst respondents tended to be scathing about the notion of formal mentors as serious influences on their careers, they did seem to appreciate their value in learning the ropes. As this system works, it shouldn't be changed. Given the apparent efficacy of informal mentoring, however, encouragement of this would probably be of value to employees (and arguably other parties). If formal methods are introduced, then mutual choice of persons in the relationship may eradicate some of the negativity of responses to current formal systems. Perhaps more realistic would be an ethos of mentoring; London and Stumpf (1986), for example, recommend that people aged 55-75 - 'young elders' - should be viewed as valuable resources, who may be encouraged to become mentors, also providing expertise in those areas in which they are not rendered technologically obsolete. The point about receptivity to mentoring suggests, however, that a more general education about the nature of work (here, the possible extrinsic benefits of social interaction) would be valuable. Perhaps such an intervention would best be made at a level of pre-vocational careers work. An accompanying ethos would need to be in existence at workplaces, however, if disillusionment is not to be the primary outcome. Such practical suggestions assume that the current research undertaken is reliable, with a broad base of external validity. Areas of further research could include similar research in other areas of the country and elsewhere, preferably with a larger and more representative survey of blue collar workers. It would be particularly desirable to know whether or not the one generalisation failure in Kram's model of mentoring processes is embedded in the British (or even Norwich) context. Analysis could also examine alternative existing models of mentoring (e.g. Schein, 1978) applied to this occupational category; new factors may have to be sought, however. Also, research should address the question of whether or not social orientation is the key to receptivity to mentoring. An alternative to, for example, factor analysis, may be to devote the bulk of a questionnaire to a variety of attributes and work attitudes which may correlate with mentor take-up. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS Clearly, the work could not have been undertaken without the participation of a large number of people totally unknown to me personally, workers in Norwich of various kinds. During a time when my confidence was low, I was particularly heartened by the interest, assistance and encouragement of Bill Edmonds, Training Officer with Norwich City Council. As my references to her in the text suggest, I was particularly helped by the ideas of Judith Coles, Proprietor of DeskToPs, Prince of Wales Road, Norwich. My tutor, Dr. Jenny Kidd, put up with the vagary of trying to timetable the equivalent of almost 18 months research into about 6 weeks. REFERENCES Bailey, K. D. (1978) Methods of Social Research. Free Press: New York. Berlew, D. E. and Hall, D. T. (1966) The socialisation of managers: effects of expectations on performance, Administrative Science Quarterly, 11, 207-223. Child, D. (1990) The Essentials of Factor Analysis. London: Cassell. Clawson, J. (1980) Mentoring in managerial careers. In Derr, C. B. (ed.), Work, Family and the Career: New Frontiers in Theory and Research. New York: Praeger. Collin, A. (1979) Notes on Some Typologies of Managerial Development and the Role of Mentor in the Process of Adaptation of the Individual to the Organisation. Personnel Review, 4, 10- 14. Dreher, G. F. and Ash, R. A. (1990) A Comparative Study of Mentoring Among Men and Women in Managerial, Professional, and Technical Positions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 75, 5, 539- 546. Erikson, E. H. (1959) Identity and the life-style. Psychological Issues, 1, 1-171. Fagenson, E. A. (1989) The Mentor advantage: perceived career/job experiences of proteges versus non-proteges. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 10, 309-320. Fagenson, E. A. (1992) Mentoring - Who Needs It? A Comparison of Proteges' and Nonproteges' Needs for Power, Achievement, Affiliation, and Autonomy. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 41, 48-60. Hofstede, G. (1980) Culture's Consequences: International Differences in Work-Related Values. Beverley Hills, Calif: Sage. Iles, P. and Mabey, C. (1993) Managerial Career Development Programmes: Effectiveness, Availability and Acceptability. British Journal of Management, 4, 103-118. Kram, K. E. (1985) Mentoring at Work. Glenview, Ill.: Scott, Foresman. Levinson, D. J., Darrow, C. N., Klein, E. B., Levinson, M. H., and McKee, B. (1978) The seasons of a man's life. New York: Knopf. London, M. and Stumpf, S. A. (1986) Individual and Organizational Career Development in Changing Times. In D. T. Hall and Associates, Career Development in Organizations, Jossey- Bass: London. Noe, R. A. (1988) An Investigation of the Determinants of Successful Assigned Mentoring Relationships. Personnel Psychology, 41, 457-479. Olian, J. D., Carroll, S. J., Giannantonio, C. M., and Feren, D. B. (1988) What Do Proteges Look for in a Mentor? Journal of Vocational Behavior, 33, 15-37. Roche, G. R. (1979) Much ado about mentors. Harvard Business Review, 57, 14-28. Scandura, T. A. (1992) Mentorship and career mobility: An empirical investigation. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 13, 169-174. Schein, E. H. (1978) Career Dynamics: matching individual and organizational needs. London: Addison-Wesley. Whiteley, W., Dougherty, T. W. and Dreher, G. F. (1992) Correlates of career-oriented mentoring for early career managers and professionals. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 13, 141- 154.
CareerSteer – careers test for career choice www.careersteer.org